The Architecture of Conversation Theory

© Copyright 1989, 2002 Paul Pangaro. All Rights Reserved.

 

 

1. Technique

The modeling technique described here is derived from Pask's Conversation Theory (CT), especially as presented in "An Approach to Machine Intelligence" (reprinted in Soft Architecture Machines, edited by Negroponte, MIT Press, 1975). There, Pask presents a formalism for describing the architecture of interactions or conversations, no matter where they may arise or among what types of entities. Because that formalism emerged from Conversation Theory, and indeed is one of its foundations, I refer to it in this paper as the CT formalism, and to the modeling that emerges from it, as detailed below, as CT modeling. (This can be misleading, because the models described here are only half the story of CT; the other half, so to speak, is that of entailment meshes, described elsewhere (PaskÕs Conversation Theory, Applications in Education and Epistemology, Amsterdam and New York: Elsevier Publishing Co., 1976., that capture the meaning that is conveyed in the conversations whose architectural structure is expressed in the models described here.)

 

The explanation that follows is couched in the example of modeling an organization or company because it was first written during a consulting project with Du Pont. One of the goals of that project, undertaken at the behest of Dr Michael C. Geoghegan, a research fellow at Du Pont at the time, was to understand the evolution of the social, political, bureaucratic, and technical conversations that were the Du Pont company. We began a modeling process, based on the formalism described below, and produced a series of snapshots of Du Pont, each at a different point in the structure of conversations. This gave us insight as to how the company had become what it had become, and also how interventions in the conversations current in the late 1980s might bring about desired change.

 

Though the modeling technique is here described with specific examples from that project, the power and beauty of Pask's work is that it can be applied to any observable interaction between participants in a conversation. This can apply in two ways:

 

  1. to the domain being modeled, whether organizational machinations alluded to above, to dialogs about learning, or interpersonal relationships in or out of crisis, or indeed any dance of relationship among language-based beings;
  2. to where the conversation is embodied, viz, to conversations among individual humans, or among groups of social systems (Republicans and Democrats, zealots and agnostics), or even within a single human, as when our "inner dialog" across multiple and often conflicting perspectives leads to new insights and evolution in our belief systems.

 

In addition to that broad scope of potential application, this modeling technique is useful because it expresses the interactions among participants in any dialog in a manner that allows deep scrutiny. Because it arises from a cybernetic sensibility, the models produced will encompass the goals, actions, information flows, feedback, and adjustments that any cybernetic model will display. The stunning power of the technique, however, lies in the insights that emerge about the "architecture" of the conversation. The diagrams capture the hierarchy of goals and actions (the objective interactions) as well as peer-to-peer language exchanges (the subjective interactions) in the same frame. As a result, complex and otherwise vague concepts, including intelligence, agreement, and misunderstanding, become specific and indeed measurable.

 

The goal of this text is to make Pask's formalism more accessible by drawing out the components via examples and expressing the dynamics in different terms than Pask's original pieces. Scope is restricted to walking through the elements of the technique and how it might be used to examine the degree of consistency in systems, and what might constitute their being "intelligent." However it must be acknowledged that this text makes only a small step in explaining the workings and power of CT modeling.

 

2. Benefits

 

For the remainder of the text, examples will be given for the case of modeling organizations, but the reader is invited to always realize that, as per the above, any conversation can be modeled using CT. Hence wherever the word "organization" appears below, the reader may substitute "system", "family", "conversation", or "person."

 

CT modeling is useful because it:

  • provides a simple and informative diagram of the observed levels and relations within an organization
  • distinguishes between interactions under the control of the organization (i.e., internal ones, performed by management fiat) and those not under direct control (i.e., external ones, negotiated with the environment)
  • produces an image of the implied as well as the expressed goals of an organization
  • gives a strict definition of "intelligent behavior", and hence allows the evaluation of any given organization against that definition
  • exposes specific organizational pathologies, such as
    • mismatch between the "managerial structure" and the working dynamic that actually controls the organization;
    • lack of action or lack of feedback, whether within levels of an organization, or between the organization and its environment
  • allows for close examination of the form of that flow of control and feedback of results
  • points to where changes must be made to maintain intelligent behavior and viability due to the evolving environment
  • predicts the potentially unsuccessful communication with other systems/ individuals/ bodies corporate
  • shows how the message may be internally inconsistent and hence misunderstood, or intentionally deceiving
  • shows how the message may be in conflict with the receiver's purposes.

The components of this general modeling technique are explained in the next section.

3. Model Diagramming

First a general overview of the two classes of interaction is given, and then details of each are diagrammed and described.

3.1.      Classes of Interaction

Interactions are modeled in two classes, each detailed in subsections following:

1.     Interactions that involve control of one process by another. Some examples are:

a.      when the organization of a corporation, in its "upper management", controls the procedures carried out by manufacturing

b.     where the corporation's control of a marketplace attempts to directly determine price or production

c.      when management controls the workers by hiring and firing at will.

These interactions are drawn vertically, and thereby represent the strict hierarchy of these relationships. In the diagrams, specific flows of control and feedback up and down the hierarchy are shown. Quite often these interactions represent what occurs only internal to a system (a and c above) though actions that attempt control of the environment are also demonstrable (b above).

2.     The second class of interactions are those in which an individual or system engages another in dialog, and where each has a say in the outcome. Some examples

a.     when a corporation negotiates with another to arrange a contract

b.     where the corporation's advertising campaign is engaging in dialog with customers at one level, and the sales process and product itself do so at others

c.      when the manager discusses a worker's future and lays out options.

 

These interactions are drawn horizontally to show that one side cannot control the other but must influence by conversation. In the diagrams, specific flows of information between participants are shown at various levels. Quite often these interactions represent what occurs across a boundary between two systems (a, b, and c just above), but further divisions within a given system (say, between divisions in a large corporation) can also be shown.

 

The next two subsections show each of these classes of interaction, vertical and horizontal, in greater detail.

 

3.2. Vertical or "it-referenced" Interactions

 

 

Figure 1

 

Figure 1 is the basic diagram of control and feedback within two given levels of an organization, showing "it-referenced" interaction that are drawn in a vertical dimension. This interaction is called "it-referenced" because the controlling process treats the controlled process like an object without choice; like an "it.Ó This is an Òit-referenced interaction whether the ÒitÓ is animate or inanimate.

 

A: "Controlling Processes (alias goal)" are, for example, management policies that are defined at this level but carried out at another. The distinction of levels is made in the course of the modeling process. The precise levels are chosen to display the flows of control and feedback that are of interest.

 

B: "Controlled Processes (alias method)" are, for example, the activities by manufacturing that carry out the goals as indicated (and dictated) by the level above.

 

C: "Injunction to execute" is the actual line of control that causes the lower level to respond, for example, the memorandum indicating start of a project or a budget authorization.

 

D: "Return of results of execution" is the actual feedback of information to the higher level, as for example a report indicating results of specific manufacturing procedures, or an internal survey.

 

E: "Comparator" is the specific mechanism whereby the feedback information is used by comparing the actual result to the desired result, or original goal.

 

F: "Iterative execution" of the entire loop takes into account the result from the comparator above, to make changes in the various processes, flows of control and feedback, etc., to make the entire loop more effective.

 

Closure occurs when comparator confirms execution of controlled processes is coherent with controlling processes (as when a goal is achieved by executing a successful method). If all of the above aspects are present (including modifications based on feedback and iterative execution, F), the system of interactions is deemed "intelligent."

 

It must be emphasized that the two levels shown are only two of possibly many vertical levels; applying CT modeling to a system of any complexity leads to multiple vertical layers in the conversation. Hence a box which appears at a "lower level" in one interaction, may itself be at the "higher level" relative to a further box that appears below it.

 

3.3. Horizontal or "I/you referenced" Interactions

 

Figure 2 shows a skeleton diagram used to express the horizontal, "I/you referenced" interactions between two given systems. This interaction is called "I/you" because there is no controlling or controlled process; each side is a participant. (It is important to note that only one direction of a completely symmetric interaction is shown.)

 

Solid lines are explicit communications, though they require interpretation by the recipient and are not perfect or unambiguous or objective. Dashed lines of the Figure indicate implied or inferred information.

Figure 2

 

The elements of the interaction are now presented, though in an arbitrary order as required by exposition:

 

G: "Communication about goal" is, for example, the communication to a customer that the company's stated "value proposition" is to provide the products with the best cost/benefit ratio, or durability, for a given application; or, to an employee, that the company considers the employee to be an essential asset for its future.

 

H: The actual result of the communication is different than what came from the "sender." ("Sender" and "receiver" are held in quotations always, to emphasize that the terms are very different from those used in information theory. However, the terms are universal and evocative and useful, and are used here with a less restricted and more powerful meaning than in information theory.) Hence "Reproduction of other's concept of goal" is a separate thing. Initially this may be taken by the receiver at face value as true and relatively well understood, though later operations may modify the situation.

 

1: "Inference of higher goal" is the production of a higher goal for which the previous communication is consistent and affirming. This is as if the "sender" had actually exchanged something at this level (shown as the upper, dashed arrow) but in fact nothing has actually been "transferred" at this level, up to this point. Quite often, the context or the common experience of the two conversants provides enough for a higher-level goal to be inferred. However, sometimes the "sender" creates a false context to encourage an incorrect inference to the "sender's" advantage, as for example when advertisers imply a food product is healthy simply because it uses the word "natural", or when a seller simply states "I have your interests at heart" while not having demonstrated this to be the case.

 

J: "Communication about method" is, for example, the communication to a customer about the details of a product's capabilities (which should affirm its stated goals, G); or, an exchange with an employee about the details of working conditions and health benefits from the corporation (which should show the method by which that employee is to be considered an asset to the corporation, relative to the goal as communicated in G).

 

K: "Reproduction of other's concept of method", as in H above, is subject to interpretation and later modification.

 

L: "Check of consistency" is a reproduction in the "receiver" of the entire loop (as per the previously described vertical interactions, see Section 3.2). This may show the consistency across the upper and lower levels, and thereby affirm understanding of the "sender's message." Of course, this can only be (at best) very close and (at worst) may only be a small fraction of the intended message. Or, the consistency check can expose the inconsistency between communicated goal and method. The "receiver" can either make queries back to the "sender" about intended meanings (not shown in the diagram); or maintain a model of the perceived inconsistency in the "sender."

 

It is ever important to be reminded that references to "goal" or "method" are relative to any pair of vertical boxes; changing level by moving up or down the hierarchy changes the attribution of "goal" or "method" for a given box. These attributions are always relative to a specific neighbor.

 

Not shown for simplicity in the Figure are potential responses, from right to left, to any given communication. Such iterative exchanges over time constitute conversation.

 

4. Checking a Model

 

It may require discipline, but the following questions should be asked at all points in the model (same labeling from Figure 2 as before):

 

A:         (Relative to a given box interpreted as a "goal") Is the level of organization well described by the name of the processes in the box at this level?

 

B:         (Relative to a lower level "method") Is the level of organization well described by the name of the processes in the box at this level?

 

C:         Does the upper level really determine (i.e., control, require, cause to occur) the execution of the processes in the lower level to which it connects? What is the mechanism that imposes that control (e.g., memorandum, budget approval)?

 

D:         Is there really a return of information from the lower level to the upper? What is the mechanism that returns a description to the upper level (e.g., internal report or survey)?

 

E:         Does the execution of the "lower" level really achieve the stated goal of the "upper" level? What is the mechanism for taking the description returned to the upper level and comparing the result to the stated goal?

 

F:          Does the entire loop get repeated and are adjustments made for improvement over time? What is the mechanism for those adjustments being made?

 

G:         Is there a communication between participants at this level? What form does it take (memo, verbal statement, legal contract, TV advertisement ... )?

 

H:         Is a (sufficiently) correct message received?

 

1:         Is the inferred higher goal consistent with the "sender's"?

 

J:          Is there a communication at this level? What form does it take?

 

K:         Is a (sufficiently) correct message received?

 

L:          Is there consistency (closure) between the communicated levels?

 

If any of the above questions A through F are answered in the negative or a mechanism is missing, the system is not intelligent and is subject to pathologies. If any of the questions G through K are answered in the negative, miscommunication has or is likely to take place. If there is no closure in L, either accidental misunderstanding or intentional miscommunication has occurred. (Of course it is possible, but less likely, that a closure in item L is inferred by the "receiver" which is not the communication of the intended message from the "sender." This can too be modeled, but only by providing an additional dimension: the content of the messages themselves.)

 

-end-